‘Human error’: The Handicap of Human Factors, Safety and Justice

“Oh my God. I told those guys at safety that it was dangerous and one day we would lose concentration and pay for it. I already told those guys at safety that it was very dangerous! We are human and this can happen to us. This curve is inhuman!”

We are human

These are the distressed words of the injured train driver moments after the train derailment in Santiago de Compostela, northern Spain on 25 July 2013. The driver can be heard pleading in sorrow, hoping for the safety of the passengers, “I have turned over. My God, my God, the poor passengers. I hope no-one is dead. I hope. I hope.” Seventy-nine people died.

In the aftermath of the accident, initial investigations ruled out mechanical or technical failure, sabotage and terrorism. That appeared to leave only two possible explanations – ‘human error’ or ‘recklessness’, or both. When society demands someone to blame, the difference – whatever it might be – can seem trivial. What followed was a display of our instinct to find a simple explanation and someone to blame. Soon, the explanation and the blame pointed to the driver. The Galicia regional government president Alberto Nunez Feijoo stated that “The driver has acknowledged his mistake”. Meanwhile, Jorge Fernandez Diaz, Spain’s Interior Minister, said that there “were reasonable grounds to think he may have a potential liability” and confirmed he could face multiple charges for reckless manslaughter. While safety investigations are ongoing, the driver faces preliminary charges of 79 counts of homicide by professional recklessness and numerous counts of bodily harm.

Several claims appeared about the driver in the media, often without relevant context. It was reported that the driver “admitted speeding” on the occasion of the crash [1]. It is known that the train was travelling at twice the speed limit on the curve and that just before the crash. The train’s black boxes showed that the train was travelling at 192 kph moments before the crash. The speed limit on the curve was 80 kph. The implication was that the speeding was reckless. The media pounced onto an old Facebook post reportedly by the driver, over a year ago, of the speeds at which his trains would travel. One post, reported by Spanish media and attributed to the driver, stated: “It would be amazing to go alongside police and overtake them and trigger off the speed camera”, accompanied by a photo of a train’s speedometer at 200 km/h (124 mph). This may be an unwise social media post, but such speeds are normal and fully permitted on the high-speed line sections.

However, there appears to be no evidence that the ‘speeding’ involved conscious disregard for, or indifference to, the dangers of the situation or for the consequences of his actions. This would have been an extreme act. Rather, it seems that the driver was unaware of the context. This hypotheses invoked ‘human error’ explanations, though carelessness was implied. It was reported that the driver himself told the judge that he was distracted and suffered a “lapse of concentration” as he approached the curve[2]. Just minutes before the derailment, the driver received a call on his work phone. The ticket inspector told El Pais that he had called the driver to instruct him to enter an upcoming station at a platform close to the station building to facilitate the exit of a family with children. The call lasted nearly two minutes; a long time when you are travelling at 192 km/h. Renfe employees are not allowed to use phones except in case of emergency, but ticket inspectors have no access to the train cab. The driver told the court he lost a sense of where the train was during the call, and believed he was on a different section of the track. It was also reported that the “driver got warnings before crash” [3], having received three warning signals. By the time he had engaged the train’s brakes, it was too late.

As is common in accidents and incidents, front-line staff immediately blame themselves, which does not mean they are to blame. Spanish press stated that immediately after the derailment, the driver allegedly said to officials at the railway station 3km from the crash “I ****** up, I want to die. So many people dead, so many people dead” [4].

In this case, the justice system will now need to determine if the driver’s actions crossed the line into ‘recklessness’. It is another issue as to whether or how justice will be served. But one only needs to look into the context of this accident to see that ‘human error’ or synonyms such as ‘lapse of concentration’ or even ‘carelessness’ do not seem reasonable to explain this terrible event. And if that is all it takes for such an outcome, then it could surely happen again. The ‘human error’ explanation does not seem to serve safety, so what does it serve? Perhaps it partly serves society’s need for simple explanations and someone to blame, while absolving society itself for its demands.

Human error or an inhuman system?

Shortly before the train crashed, according to reports, the Spanish train had passed from a computer-controlled area of the track to a zone that requires the driver to take control of braking and deceleration. Furthermore, there was no automatic braking system on the curve in question. The European Rail Traffic Management System automatic braking program was installed on most of the high-speed track but stopped 3 miles south of where the crash occurred. This placed responsibility on the driver significantly to reduce speed at a crucial time. The sharp bend was known to be “dangerous” and has previously been subject to debates and warnings. According to Spanish journalist Miguel-Anxo Murado, “There were arguments for having that section of the route remade completely, but Galicia’s particular land tenure regime makes expropriations an administrative nightmare. So the bend was left as it was, and speed was limited there to 80km/h.” The driver’s recorded phone call indicated that he knew this and had foretold such an accident in a warning to the company’s safety specialists: “I already told those guys at safety that it was very dangerous. We are human and this can happen to us. This curve is inhuman.”. The judge is now reportedly expanding the preliminary charges to include numerous top officials of the state railway infrastructure company, Adif, including rail safety senior officials, for alleged negligence [5].

Reminiscent of the Chernobyl inquiry, a small number of media reports broadened the focus to what might be called reckless expansion in society more generally: “I can’t help feeling that, at some profound or superficial moral level, we also played our part in the tragedy as a society; that this was the last, most tragic episode of a decade of oversized dreams, fast money and fast trains”, said journalist Miguel-Anxo Murado [6]. If this stretches the argument, it at least gives a counterbalance to the ‘human error’ or ‘recklessness’ explanations of this tragic event.

The psychology of error / The error of psychology

There are thousands of pages of research in the psychology and human factors literature on the issues mentioned so far. The ‘reversion to manual’ problem has been studied extensively in the context of automation and manual operation. The distracting effects of phone calls – hands free or not – are well-documented. ‘Multitasking’ is known to have devastating effects on performance, yet conflicts between safety and efficiency goals often demand that we switch from one task to another in a given timeframe. There are thousands of articles on situation awareness along with many books. The same is true of safety culture, including how organisations respond to safety concerns.

But ‘human error’ has been a fascination of psychologists for over a hundred years. Psychology is a scientific discipline concerned the mind and behaviour, and therefore tends to have an individual or social focus. For decades, human mishaps have been dissected and further dissected into multiple categories in the scientific literature. Well-known names including James Reason and Don Norman were early pioneers of the study of error, and developed psychological explanations for slips and lapses via individual diary and laboratory studies (Reason, 1979[7]; Norman, 1981[8]). Mistakes were subsequently studied, and ‘violations’ followed (see Reason’s landmark ‘Human Error’, 1990 [9]). Human factors (or ergonomics), meanwhile, is a design discipline concerned with interactions in socio-technical systems. Knowledge concerning people and complex safety-critical systems has been applied to real systems in most industries to avoid, reduce or mitigate human error.

Indeed, the popularisation of the term ‘human error’ has provided perhaps the biggest spur to the development of human factors in safety-related industries – with a downside. When something goes wrong, complexity is reduced to this simple, pernicious, term. ‘Human error’ has become a shapeshifting persona that can morph into an explanation of almost any unwanted event. It is now almost guaranteed to be found in news stories pertaining to major accidents. Interestingly, some reports specify that human error was not the cause. The reverse implication being that human error would otherwise have been the cause (e.g. “Paris train crash: human error not to blame”, Telegraph, 13/07/13). Since the term suffices as explanation, little or no mention of findings in psychology or human factors, including the context and conditions of performance, is required.

This is very unsatisfactory to many psychologists; the implication in research and practice was that human error is ‘normal’ – it is part of who we are. Similarly, it is very unsatisfactory to many human factors specialists who try to predict and design for error. But in the context of safety and in justice, ‘human error’ has been taken to mean something different – a deviation from normal, from rules, procedures, regulations and laws.

The demise of error

Despite decades of research, there has been little agreement on the precise meaning of the term, and more recently whether it has any real meaning at all. While the term may have some value in simple systems and situations, there are problems with the use of the term in complex systems such as ATC. These are now well documented in the literature. While ‘human error’ is still the explanation of choice for accidents, the term itself fell into disrepute among some thinkers more than a decade ago [10, 11].

After being fascinated by the concept of human error since encountering it while studying psychology in the early 1990s, I gradually and reluctantly accepted these arguments in the first few years of the 2000s. Reading the works of Erik Hollnagel, Sidney Dekker, David Woods, Rene Amalberti and others, I grew increasingly uncomfortable with the concept and term. This inconveniently coincided with the final stages of a PhD in human error in air traffic control. My own realisation finally crystallised when reviewing Erik Hollnagel’s book ‘Barriers and accidents prevention’ in 2004 [12]. I committed to abandoning the term. My own reasons followed the arguments of those mentioned above (presented to the Safety and Reliability Society in 2006).

  • ‘Human error’ is a often a post hoc social judgement. ‘Human error’ is one of few things that often cannot be defined unambiguously in advance of it happening.
  • ‘Human error’ requires a standard. To know that something is an error, it must be possible to describe a non-error. This can be surprisingly difficult, partly because there are so many “it depends”. In the context of complex interacting systems such as ATC, there are many ways to get an acceptable result.
  • ‘Human error’ points to individuals in a complex system. In complex systems, system behaviour is driven fundamentally by the goals of the system and the system structure. People provide the flexibility to make it work.
  • ‘Human error’ stigmatises actions that could have been heroic in slightly different circumstances. What are described as heroic actions could often have been described as tragic errors if the circumstances were only slightly different. The consequences of heroic actions are not known in advance.
  • Underlying processes of ‘human error’ are often vital for task performance. In the context of error, we often refer to psychological activity involved in perception, memory, decision making or action. Taking one example, without expectation, radio-telephony would be very inefficient. Occasionally, one may hear what one expects instead of what is said, but this must be set against improved efficiency during thousands of other occasions.
  • ‘Human error’ is an inevitable by-product of the pursuit of successful performance in a variable world. The context and conditions of performance are often vague, shifting and suboptimal. The ability to adapt and compensate comes at a cost.

Still, the term ‘human error’ is used frequently in human factors and psychology, and practitioners reside in several camps. The first camp continues to use the term with ‘good intent’ and add caveats that human error is normal and we need to talk about it in order to learn from it. But in doing so, they risk sounding like Humpty Dumpty in Lewis Carroll’s ‘Through the Looking Glass’ (“’When I use a word,’ Humpty Dumpty said, in rather a scornful tone, ‘it means just what I choose it to mean — neither more nor less.’”). A related second camp appears reluctantly to use the term for convenience but at the same time rejects the simplistic concept and argues that the term refers to a symptom of deeper organisational troubles. A third camp has abandoned the use of the term, except reflexively, to refer to the term itself. This latter camp perhaps recognises that the term itself is damaging. Personally, I have moved from camp one, through camp two, and finally to camp three. While psychology and human factors did not intend some of the simplistic meanings ascribed to the term, the genie is out of the bottle.

“Don’t call me handicapped!”

Over roughly the same period as the demise of ‘human error’ in scientific discourse, the term ‘handicap’ became seen as offensive in some English-speaking countries [13]. The word itself replaced other terms that were seen as offensive. One reason the word ‘handicap’ is sometimes considered offensive is because it has been mistakenly associated with the phrase ‘cap in hand’, referring to beggars. This is a false etymology. The elaborate version of the story is that in 1504, after a brutal war in England, King Henry VII passed legislation that begging in the streets be legal for people with disabilities. In fact, handicap was shortened from ‘hand in cap’; a game played in the 1600s with two players and a referee that combined elements of barter and lottery. The game involved equalising the value of an exchange.

The word grew to refer to any action that worked to make a contest more equitable. In sports, the term was used to make competitions fairer, so that the stronger contestants bore a penalty. From 1754 the word was used to describe horse races where weights were added under the saddle of a faster horses. Subsequently, faster runners were made to start behind slower runners. The word evolved further to mean a physical limitation, first used in 1915 in the context of children. People of older generations may still use the word ‘handicapped’, and use it with good intent. But in several Anglophone countries the term is unwanted and seen as unhelpful in any of its meanings. It has been replaced by ‘disabled people’ and ‘people with disabilities’. Different terms have different connotations and encourage a different way of thinking.

‘Human error’ as handicap

Perhaps ‘human error’ has become the handicap of human factors. Semantically, both terms have multiple meanings that have taken different evolutionary paths. ‘Human error’ as used nowadays in the media, and even by many safety specialists, often implies causality and agency (even guilt) with reference to adverse events. While the terms may be used with good intent by some, the plaintiff cry “That’s not what we mean!” cannot undo modern connotations.

Metaphorically, just as weights were used in handicap racing to weigh down or limit a horse, ‘human error’ has limited the lay appreciation and application of human factors by focusing on the ‘human factor’, rather than socio-technical system interactions, which is the real focus of human factors. In accident models, ‘human error’ is usually close to the consequences, encouraging the logical fallacy “post hoc ergo propter hoc” (“after this, therefore because of this”). By limiting the evolution of human factors, ‘human error’ also limits our understanding of safety, and the term is captured by the legal system and translated to carelessness, or worse.

Socially, as the term ‘handicap’ is potentially stigmatising of disabled people or people with disabilities, the term ‘human error’ is stigmatising of people caught up in systems failures, even if some ‘mitigating circumstances’ (such as fatigue) are permitted.

Perhaps most importantly, both terms imply deviation from ‘normal’ or an ideal. In the case of ‘human error’, for complex tasks such as air traffic control there is often no normal or ideal that can be precisely and exactly described (see Hollnagel, 2009). As is visible after only a few hours observing and talking to air traffic controllers, what controllers actually do depends on many things. These include traffic demand and the context and conditions, such as staffing in the ops room, who you are working with, the state of the procedures, the shift system, and the equipment in and out of the ops room. In fact, work by Chris Johnson on degraded modes of operation [14] suggests that ‘normal operation’ is in fact abnormality; we get used to operating in various degraded modes of operation. This means that people must make continuous adjustments in order to adapt and respond to the context and work demands. What can be expected is variability and diversity, not deviation from a standard.

Words shape worlds

Does it all matter, if we still use the term ‘human error’ when we know what we mean? Do we risk falling onto a euphemism treadmill, skipping from one term to the next? [15] The argument presented here is that it does matter. Our language affects the way we view the world and how we approach problems. Even if we know what we mean when we talk about ‘human error’, and even if it does seem to fit our everyday slip-ups and blunders in life, the term reinforces unwanted connotations, especially when we are talking about high-hazard systems. While we cannot put the genie of human error back in the bottle, we can use a new vocabulary to create a new understanding.

Left with a ‘human error’-shaped hole in my vocabulary several years ago, I found an alternative concept thanks to Erik Hollnagel: performance variability. This is not simply a replacement term or a euphemism, but a new way of thinking that acknowledges how systems really work. Performance variability, both at an individual level and at a system or organisational level, is both normal and necessary, and it is mostly deliberate. What controllers actually do varies, because it has to. We have to make efficiency-thoroughness trade-offs, as well as other tradeoffs. This flexibility is why humans are required to do the job. Also, people naturally have different preferred styles of working and there are several ways to do the same job. There is of course some leftover unwanted variability – you can’t have one without the other. But without performance variability, success would not be possible. It is not the aim of this article to explain this in more detail, but the reader is encouraged to explore this further (see Hollnagel, 2009).

Image

Performance variability is not simply a replacement term but a new way of thinking that acknowledges how systems really work. Photo: Kevin Dooley CC BY 2.0

More generally, if we wish to understand how systems really work, and improve how they work, we need to enrich our vocabulary with systems concepts – and use them in preference of simplistic terms that don’t help explain how systems actually function. This is not to say that people are not responsible for their actions – of course they are. What is relevant is the difference between normal variability in human performance, and what we define as recklessness. Labeling either as ‘human error’ is not helpful.

Folks, it’s time to evolve ideas

Human error’ has long outlived its usefulness in systems safety, and has now become the handicap of human factors, safety and justice. We can’t expect society to change the way it thinks and talks about systems and safety if we continue in the same old way. It’s time to evolve ideas and think in systems, but for that to happen, our language must change. Overcoming ‘human error’ in our language is the first hurdle.

Further reading

Dekker, S.W.A., (2006). The field guide to understanding human error. Ashgate.

Hollnagel, E. (2009). The ETTO principle: Efficiency-thoroughness trade-off. Ashgate.

Meadows, D. (2009). Thinking in systems. Routledge.


[1] Spain train crash driver admits speeding in emergency call recording, Telegraph, 06/09/13
[2] Spain train crash: Driver told judge he was ‘distracted’, Telegraph, 06/09/13
[3] Spanish train wreck driver got warnings before crash, Reuters, 02/0813
[4] ‘Reckless’ Train Crash Driver Held By Police, Sky NEws, 26/07/13
[5] Train crash judge summons track safety managers, Leader, 10/09/13
[6] Spain train crash: human error over decades, not just seconds, Guardian, 25 July 2013
[7] Reason, J. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness: Vol. 1. Psychological issues. London:Wiley.[8] Norman, D.A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review, 88, 1–15.
[9] Reason, J. (1990). Human error. Cambridge University Press.
[10] Hollnagel, E. and Amalberti, R. (2001). The Emperor’s New Clothes, or whatever happened to “human error”? Invited keynote presentation at 4th International Workshop on Human Error, Safety and System Development. Linköping, June 11–12, 2001.
[11] Dekker, S.W.A., (2006). The field guide to understanding human error. Ashgate.
[12] Hollnagel, E. (2004). Barriers and accidents prevention. Ashgate.
[13] Don’t call me handicapped! BBC News, 4 October, 2004.
[14] See http://www.skybrary.aero/bookshelf/books/1055.pdf
[15] This risk, and the comparison with terms for disability, was pointed out to me by a human factors colleague, which prompted this article.

Author: stevenshorrock

This blog is written by Dr Steven Shorrock. I am interdisciplinary humanistic, systems and design practitioner interested in human work from multiple perspectives. My main interest is human and system behaviour, mostly in the context of safety-related organisations. I am a Chartered Ergonomist and Human Factors Specialist with the CIEHF and a Chartered Psychologist with the British Psychological Society. I currently work as a human factors and safety specialist in air traffic control in Europe. I am also Adjunct Associate Professor at University of the Sunshine Coast, Centre for Human Factors & Sociotechnical Systems. I blog in a personal capacity. Views expressed here are mine and not those of any affiliated organisation, unless stated otherwise. You can find me on twitter at @stevenshorrock or email contact[at]humanisticsystems[dot]com.

13 thoughts

  1. Haven’t seen you tie human error and the need to find the culprit with regard to American Jurisprudence, and who should pay. How does Europe compensate victims and families?

  2. Good post. Performance variability is an interesting way of looking at things, certainly in a proactive arena. I’m not sure it would entirely work as a term in what I do, reactive investigations – although it would have value. But the train driver was right. If there is scope for error, at some point, that error will occur.

  3. How about the limits of deliberate attention? In World War 2 didn’t they rotate radar operators every 15 mins, and didn’t William James write extensively about this in the 1890’s?

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out /  Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out /  Change )

Connecting to %s

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.